• We have updated the guidelines regarding posting political content: please see the stickied thread on Website Issues.

Forensic Science / Tech: Limitations & Flaws

Mighty_Emperor

Gone But Not Forgotten
(ACCOUNT RETIRED)
Joined
Aug 18, 2002
Messages
19,407
Terror-dactylography? Or someone who could make a jolly good living acting as an expert witness for the defence?

Posted on Wed, Oct. 27, 2004

Fingerprint evidence not good science, scholar says

BY EDWARD HUMES

The Orange County Register

SANTA ANA, Calif. - (KRT) - Clarence Hiller's wife awoke with a start and shook him from sleep, sensing something amiss before she realized what it was: The room was too dark. The hallway light outside their daughter's room, the one they always left on at night, had gone out. And so the dutiful husband and father of four rose from bed to have a look, and to begin the last 30 seconds of his life.

Near the top of the stairs Hiller met an unexpected obstacle: the shadowy form of a man. The intruder lunged, and the two men, shoving and flailing, thudded down the stairs. At the landing, the stranger wrestled a pistol free and shot Hiller twice. Then the man who had merely wanted to put on the night-light died with his wife's screams still in the air, the burglar-turned-murderer pounding out the door and into the darkness.

The Chicago police soon arrived and pieced together what happened. The killer had climbed through the Hillers' kitchen window, entering quietly but leaving behind a key bit of evidence: On a freshly painted railing beneath the window, investigators found the vivid imprints of four fingers of someone's left hand embedded in the still-soft paint.

At 2:38 that morning, the police spotted Thomas Jennings, a paroled burglar, limping through the darkness not far from the Hiller home. They frisked him and found a revolver in his pocket. None of the Hillers got a good look at his face, but it made little difference. Four police experts agreed that, without doubt, Jennings' left hand had grabbed that wooden rail, a clean, simple, case-closing resolution - and a seminal moment in law-enforcement history.

For nearly a century, that fateful encounter on a staircase in 1910 has both empowered and haunted the justice system, as Clarence Hiller's killer became the first person in America to be arrested, convicted, sentenced to death, and hanged through the triumph of dactylography, the analysis of fingerprints.

So when Simon Cole, a social sciences professor at the University of California, Irvine, wages his lonely campaign to prove that the science of fingerprint analysis is missing a crucial element - namely, the science part - he isn't merely going up against the police, the FBI and pretty much every black robe in the land. Cole is taking on Clarence Hiller's ghost.

Hiller's case and the legal logic surrounding it ushered in a new era of modern law enforcement, making possible the millions of fingerprint comparisons and cases that followed, from those used in the Lindbergh kidnapping to the prosecution of the al-Qaida operative who planned the millennium bombing of LAX.

And this drives Cole absolutely crazy.

According to Cole - and a growing number of scientists, scholars and legal experts horrified at some recent, high-profile fingerprint blunders - the courts have gotten it wrong for the last 94 years. They have been wrong, Cole says, not because Jennings was innocent, but because he really was guilty - lending a lasting aura of invincibility to fingerprint analysis that it has neither earned nor deserved.

---

Since Clarence Hiller's murder, the legal system has treated fingerprint comparisons as not simply invaluable, which they unquestionably are, but as essentially infallible - when they are anything but.

Just ask Brandon Mayfield, the Oregon lawyer who was arrested in May, jailed for two weeks and branded a terrorist. FBI experts mistakenly linked his fingerprint to the Madrid train bombing that killed 191 people in March - even though Mayfield has never been to Madrid. The fingerprints were later found by Spanish experts to match a foreign terrorist.

FBI officials have apologized for the error, but they have not yet explained it - which is no surprise to Cole, who has made UCI ground zero for challenging conventional wisdom about fingerprints.

"They can't explain it, because there is a fallacy at work here: the belief that, because all fingerprints are unique, therefore fingerprint evidence is inherently reliable," he says. "It makes sense at first blush, but think about it: No two faces are alike, yet eyewitness identification is difficult and problem-plagued.

"The real question is not whether all fingerprints are different, but how accurate are fingerprint examiners at matching the small, fragmentary prints you find at crime scenes. And the real answer is, we don't know. No one knows - because there has never been a scientific study to find out. They have never allowed it."

Rather grudgingly, and with many caveats, several leading fingerprint examiners concede that Cole is correct: There have been no formal scientific studies on the reliability of fingerprint examiners or the frequency of errors like Madrid.

"No, the science nerds have never actually studied it," says Pat Wertheim, a nationally prominent fingerprint examiner with the Arizona Department of Public Safety in Tucson. "But you can't ignore 79 years of empirical research."

Seventy-nine years is how long the FBI has been in the fingerprint business, and by "empirical research," Wertheim means the day-to-day use of fingerprint evidence by police agencies and its acceptance by the courts. Cole says this proves nothing but his own point: "Legal opinions are not scientific studies. Judges are notorious for being scientifically illiterate."

Long dismissed for his views, Cole's arguments have gained steam, if not legal impact, in the wake of the FBI's embarrassment over the Madrid case. Three top FBI fingerprint experts mistakenly matched Mayfield (who happens to be a convert to Islam) to a print found on bomb-making materials found in Madrid.

The errors were all the more humiliating because the FBI had in the last two years successfully headed off several legal challenges to fingerprint evidence by convincing federal judges that its procedures had never yielded such a mistake, and that Cole's concerns were of no consequence. Several wrongful convictions have also come to light involving fingerprint matches by local police.

Now the subject of fingerprint evidence's lack of scientific foundation has provoked an angry editorial in the respected journal Science, in which Donald Kennedy, formerly Stanford University president and Food and Drug Administration commissioner, called forensic science "an oxymoron." A number of prominent scientists and legal scholars have joined the fray.

Fingerprint examiners are so outraged and concerned by the growing legion of critics that they have sponsored symposia and professional newsletter article series, including one entitled "Defense Against the Dark Arts" (a reference to juvenile literary hero Harry Potter's battles against evil wizardry), in order to pass on strategies to use against Cole and the other critics in court.

FBI officials, on the other hand, have imposed an in-house gag order, declining to comment for this article or to permit interviews with their fingerprint experts - something that was routinely permitted in the past.

The stakes could not be higher in this debate: If Cole is correct, a strict interpretation of federal law could theoretically bar the use of fingerprint evidence pretty much everywhere until studies are performed to see how regularly fingerprint comparisons go wrong.

"Simon's been predicting something like Madrid would happen," says Cole's UCI colleague, William Thompson, who has fought similar battles over DNA fingerprinting. "He's been saying this was inevitable. Now, finally, people are beginning to listen."

The professors in the law, crime and psychology group at UCI's School of Social Ecology speak ruefully of the wars they've fought over their research on science and the justice system, which often defies the pronouncements of authorities.

With leading experts in eyewitness errors, recovered memory, the testimony of children and the phenomenon of false confessions, UCI's reputation as a center for research on criminal justice has drawn international attention.

---

Cole's fingerprint war is only the latest in a trend begun by Thompson, widely regarded as one of the nation's leading legal experts on DNA and crime-lab errors; his work includes a takedown of the Los Angeles Police Department lab in the O.J. Simpson case, a scathing indictment of rampant errors in Houston's crime lab, and winning the freedom of an innocent Texas 16-year-old sentenced to 25 years for rape based on an erroneous DNA match.

"The technology works," Thompson says. "The problem is human error."

Now this same balance of technology and human judgment is in question with fingerprints. The 25-year history of DNA matching and the century-long saga of fingerprint evidence are remarkably similar: Both were introduced as triumphs of science, both were received as magic bullets for police and prosecutors to solve previously unsolvable crimes. But something happened with DNA evidence that has never occurred with fingerprints: successful legal challenges, which fueled reform and improvements.

After DNA technology was introduced in court cases in the late '80s, Thompson and other attorneys nationwide found many highly regarded scientific experts had uncovered errors and scientific uncertainties in the DNA matching methods used by police and the FBI.

Competing experts accused one another of malfeasance, harassment and scientific fraud. In those days, Thompson, who works as both an attorney and a consultant on DNA cases, was the target of personal attacks from some prosecutors, who labeled him and a colleague as "The Combine from Irvine." These DNA Wars peaked in the 1990s, when several jurisdictions in California briefly barred DNA evidence because of errors and misleading statistics.

In the end, the legal challenges on behalf of criminal defendants have achieved an unintended purpose: They have led to hundreds of government-backed scientific studies of DNA evidence, to improved procedures and training in police labs, and to more scientifically valid presentations of the meaning of DNA evidence in court.

Now most experts agree that the technology is sound, and defense lawyers rely on DNA evidence nearly as often as prosecutors. Most remaining challenges to DNA evidence, such as those in Houston, revolve not around the underlying science, but on shoddy lab work in individual cases, poor training of analysts and a lack of national standards for crime labs that have left some DNA testing operations virtual paragons while others, some government-run, others privately run, are considered virtual pariahs.

The same sort of skeptical inquiry and scientific rigor unleashed on DNA evidence, Cole and Thompson say, has never been applied to fingerprint matches. The distinction seems particularly clear in how differently the courts treat fingerprint analysts versus every other kind of forensic expert.

As powerful as DNA evidence can be, experts generally are not allowed to say that a particular defendant's DNA "matches" a DNA sample connected to a crime. They can say the samples are consistent. They can say that such DNA patterns occur only in a tiny portion of the human population, and that the defendant falls into that small group. But they cannot point the finger at a defendant and say, "That's his DNA."

The same is true of experts in hair, fiber, paint, blood spatter, bullet comparisons, arson and just about any other forensic comparison imaginable. These experts cannot pronounce a person guilty - that's the jury's job. The courts recognize that each one of these types of forensic evidence has to be interpreted through subjective human judgment.

Except, say Cole and Thompson, for fingerprint experts. They alone are allowed to say: The prints belong to the defendant. Not "consistent with" the defendants. Not just that they have similar loops and whorls. They can utter the words no other forensic expert can use: They match.

In other words, as Cole sees it, fingerprint experts are so exalted by the courts that they can, in many cases, pronounce guilt for a jury.

"I don't know about you," says Cole. "But I have a problem with that."

Cole wants to take a page from the defense lawyer's DNA playbook and use it to change the way fingerprint evidence is handled in court. Fingerprints would be subjected to the same scientifically rigorous examination used for all other forensic evidence - even if it is 94 years late.

Fingerprint experts have fought back, sometimes angrily, maintaining that their methods have been proven in the real world of criminal investigations, which they deem more important than strict scientific testing.

They accuse Cole of lacking credentials in fingerprint analysis (something he cheerfully concedes) and of a desire to sell books rather than see justice done (which he not-so-cheerfully denies). This last point is a reference to Cole's 2001 book on the history of fingerprint evidence, "Suspect Identities: A History of Fingerprinting and Criminal Identification." His critics in the forensic community are well-represented on the comments section at Amazon.com, where one reviewer even quoted an obscure legal opinion from a trial court in New York that rejected Cole's status as an expert witness and branded his assertions "junk science."

If there were a chalkboard tallying scores in this debate, Cole would be the underdog. He has convinced many scientists that there is cause for concern, but the courts have always sided with the fingerprint examiners. Now, the Madrid case put his critics on the defensive. The Fingerprint War, like the DNA War that preceded it, is suddenly in full swing.

Fingerprints, the prosaic stuff of every B-movie murder and rerun of "Barnaby Jones," represented a revolution when the handcuffs snapped onto Tom Jennings' wrists in 1910. The discovery that fingerprints could be read in a way that rendered people as unique as snowflakes did as much for crime detection as penicillin would do three decades later for health care.

The scientific reasoning, factual findings, even the wording of the legal opinion on the reliability of fingerprint evidence in the Jennings case have been quoted, paraphrased and depended upon ever since.

How the ancient Egyptians used Pharaoh's thumbprint as an official identifier. How contract signers and courts in India for decades relied upon fingerprints as binding identifications. How the system of fingerprint analysis invented by Sir Francis Galton, cousin to Charles Darwin, had a firm scientific basis and, by 1911, had been used by the British police in "thousands of cases without error."

"This method of identification is in such general and common use that the courts cannot refuse to take judicial cognizance of it," the Illinois Supreme Court boldly concluded in the Jennings case, reciting what seemed to be, even then, a long and solidly established history of dactylography. And in the years that followed, every state, federal and local court has agreed in untold millions of cases, embracing fingerprint comparisons as a kind of forensic Holy Grail that has withstood the test of time.

The problem is, none of the original reasoning, which has been relied upon by the courts ever since, holds water:

The ancient Egyptians didn't use fingerprints as identifiers, but as royal seals, no more relevant to the case than hieroglyphics.

The practice of using fingerprints on contracts in India was not based on science, but on superstition, undertaken on a whim by a British magistrate who felt his Indian subjects would fear their personal imprint on a document was more mystically binding than a mere signature.

As for Galton, he did invent a revolutionary method of analyzing the loops and whorls of fingerprints that is still in use today, though he conceived of it not as a crime-busting tool, but as a means of classifying the genetically superior among us. Galton, in his most-remembered work, was the father of eugenics, his theory of controlled breeding and noble birth that has been used over the years to justify such human travesties as forced sterilizations, pogroms, genocide and the Holocaust.

And as for those thousands of cases in Britain in which fingerprints were used without error prior to 1911? The truth is, Cole says, no one knew then if there were any errors. The police experts were simply taken at their word. "Just like today," Cole says.

"Fingerprint comparison may have withstood the test of time but that's the only test it has passed," Cole said. "Personally, I don't find the test of time very persuasive. The flat-Earth theory withstood the test of time for a few thousand years, but that didn't make it correct."

Although he is vilified alternately as ignorant, arrogant or maniacal on Web sites frequented by forensic analysts, Cole, 37, seems an unlikely character to be leading this charge. He is neither a laboratory scientist nor a lawyer. He describes himself instead as a social scientist and science historian, an easygoing and affable scholar who, these days, seems perpetually tired and disheveled - the sleepless look every father of a newborn baby (Cole's second) knows all too well.

"He has irritated a few folks," Wertheim says tactfully. "Professor Cole is a very sincere man, but misguided."

Cole's consuming interest in fingerprint evidence developed five years ago, the inadvertent consequence of a study he conducted at Rutgers University in New Jersey. He had been investigating why jurors, lawyers and judges found fingerprint examiners with only a few months of training so overwhelmingly credible, while DNA experts with far more impressive training were being picked apart on the stand. "I found that you just couldn't do that with fingerprint examiners," Cole recalls. "They were essentially unassailable."

At the time, Cole chalked this up to the clever way fingerprint images are used in court: The experts would show jurors blow-ups of the fingerprints and point out similarities between a crime-scene "latent" print and an inked fingerprint from a defendant. When these similarities proved hard for a layperson to perceive - as they often are - Cole found that fingerprint examiners were adept at explaining this away by asserting that only a trained expert could make such determinations.

"It was a Catch-22," Cole says. "Whether you see a similarity or not, the fingerprint examiner has an explanation for why it's a match."

Add to that decades of positive publicity Americans have received through television, film and books - dating back to Mark Twain's "Pudd'nhead Wilson" - that extolled the virtues of fingerprinting, and Cole found a ready explanation for why fingerprint analysts are esteemed above all other courtroom experts.

He might have left it at that but for a call he received a short time later from a public defender in Philadelphia, who was mounting the first serious legal challenge under a then-new federal rule for admitting scientific evidence in court, the Daubert Test.

Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals had been a product-liability case in which the parents of two children born with serious birth defects sued the drug maker, blaming the defects on the anti-nausea drug Bendectin. In 1993, the Supreme Court helped resolve the case by crafting a new legal test for scientific evidence, tossing out old courtroom standards that considered only whether a scientific principle was generally accepted. The new Daubert Test required a more detailed judicial inquiry by trial judges, including an examination of likely error rates in any scientific procedure

The new standard, which applies to all federal courts and has been adopted by some states, applied to criminal cases as well as lawsuits, and soon uncovered wholesale mistakes and frauds by some crime labs, with most of the errors helpful to prosecutors. A serologist in West Virginia had falsified hundreds of tests to help sustain convictions. A pathologist had faked autopsy results in more than 20 death-penalty cases. Even the FBI was found to have a host of lab problems.

A side effect of this new rule was an opening to re-examine the reliability of fingerprint evidence.

To Cole's astonishment, once he agreed to join the case as an expert witness, he found virtually no scientific information to back up the claims of 100-percent accuracy of fingerprint examiners.

There were studies that purported to show everyone's fingerprints were unique, but nothing to prove a human fingerprint examiner's ability to match fingerprints or, more importantly, partial crime-scene fingerprints.

"That call changed my life," says Cole. "I've been at it ever since."

---

The public defender who called Cole eventually lost his bid to throw out fingerprint evidence. But another federal judge in Philadelphia took on the issue a short time later, again using testimony from Cole and others, and reached the opposite conclusion. He found in a drug case against a man named Llera Plaza that the critics of fingerprint evidence were right, and that fingerprint examiners, while they should be allowed to testify, should be barred from claiming that a particular person absolutely matched a particular print. More scientific study was needed, U.S. District Judge Louis Pollack ruled in 2002, before the luster could be restored to fingerprint matching.

Fingerprint examiners nationwide were flabbergasted. If sustained on appeal, it could undo hundreds, if not thousands, of criminal cases, they feared. The FBI mounted a concerted effort at overturning the decision, bringing in a legion of experts to argue that, in 79 years, it had never made a false match, that fingerprint errors discussed by Cole and others had been made by police agencies other than the FBI, and that Pollack's decision would have disastrous consequences for public safety.

In a rare move three months later, Pollack overturned the ruling, saying he had been wrong in his initial decision and that the FBI could continue to make fingerprint identifications in court.

Cole moved to the UCI faculty a few months later and, since then, has testified in hearings in which the scientific reliability of fingerprint evidence has been questioned, all with the same result: The evidence was deemed admissible. Cole's arguments lost.

Yet the war continues, because fingerprint errors keep cropping up. Fingerprint examiners argue that errors cannot occur when proper methods are used. These methods include examining every fingerprint match for differences rather than similarities, then having multiple fingerprint examiners confirm every match.

"When a fingerprint examination is performed correctly, that is, following the accepted methodology, there are no misidentifications," according to Cindy Homer, a fingerprint expert for the state of Maine, who cautioned her colleagues in a Web posting to welcome criticism from Cole and others as an opportunity to stay sharp. "We don't know yet what actually went wrong with Mayfield. I can say because there were misidentifications, the fingerprint-analysis methodology was not followed correctly."

Cole takes issue with that position, which is nearly universal among fingerprint analysis, because it is unproven. Simply saying that whenever there is a mistake, it must be because procedures weren't followed is an argument, Cole says. But it is not proof.

Without a thorough study of error rates by fingerprint examiners who are following proper procedures, Cole says, there is no way to know what causes errors, or how common or uncommon they are. The few proficiency tests that fingerprint examiners have participated in have had mixed results: One 1995 battery of tests given to examiners nationwide found a one-in-five error rate. But instead of leading to a push for more such tests, Cole says, there has been little follow-up.

Several recent fingerprint blunders seem to bolster Cole's position. Richard Jackson of Upper Darby, Pa., was sentenced to life without parole and served two years before his lawyers convinced a judge that local police had mistakenly matched his fingerprints to the killer's. A re-examination exonerated him, but two of the three police experts who made the error and verified one another's erroneous matches are still doing fingerprint analysis.

In Boston, Stephan Cowans was released earlier this year after serving seven years in prison for shooting a police officer. He was convicted based on fingerprint evidence that had been reviewed by at least two police analysts. Later, DNA testing exonerated him. A re-examination of the fingerprint once again showed there was no match, despite the verification procedures.

The Cowans case was soon followed by the arrest of attorney Mayfield in connection with the Madrid bombing, after three senior FBI examiners, the most highly trained fingerprint experts in the country, mistakenly declared his prints a match to the bomb suspect's partial, blurred latent print.

The error was compounded when a nationally renowned fingerprint expert hired by the defense also found that Mayfield's prints matched the bomber's. The match, then, was verified four times - incorrectly.

The error might never have been detected at all if the bombing occurred in this country, Cole argues.

Only the additional work by the Spanish police revealed the mismatch.

He believes this case alone should shake the legal system's belief in fingerprint examiner infallibility.

"If the case had been entirely in the U.S., Mayfield would still be in jail," Cole says. "All the proper procedures would have been followed and verified, and an innocent man would be facing the death penalty."

The FBI has convened an international panel of experts to study and report on what went wrong in the Mayfield case, but has remained silent since issuing an apology.

"I don't understand what happened with that case - we are all waiting to hear," Homer says. "This case has left the fingerprint community scratching our collective heads."

Cole believes the problem can be explained this way: Fingerprint examiners, even top ones, can err when the pressure is great and the partial fingerprints are spotty, as they were in the Madrid case. Yet their findings are viewed as so persuasive and so overwhelming that even other fingerprint examiners are psychologically affected and tend to verify a declared match when they know other examiners they trust has found one.

On the other hand, Cole says, once doubts were raised by the Spanish authorities, other experts, went online, took a look at the Mayfield prints, decided they obviously did not match, and then expressed bewilderment that there had been any suspicion of Mayfield to begin with.

Surprisingly, Cole, Wertheim and Homer agree on more than they realize. All three say national standards and better training are crucial to curtail erroneous fingerprint matches - and are common in other countries. All say they would like to see more scientific studies, and all say there has been almost no funding available to conduct such inquiries.

Where they part company is what to do about it. Fingerprint examiners say they have to be allowed to continue testifying about absolute matches, and they feel the courts have ratified their position time and again. Research by forensic experts to answer Cole'sand others' criticisms is under way now, but it is hampered by a lack of research money, the examiners say.

Cole says a growing sentiment among scientists suggests fingerprint evidence should be viewed far more skeptically, and that it should be reined in if not blocked until proper studies are conducted. As with DNA evidence, court-imposed restrictions would quickly drive improvements, Cole says.

A comprehensive study of error rates would probably show a small but currently undetected number of mistaken matches are made each year.

But if that means one false imprisonment - or one wrongful death sentence - each year or every two years or even once every decade, the system needs to correct it, Cole says.

But it won't come easy, he adds - the war will continue for the foreseeable future.

"No judge wants to say that we admitted fingerprint matches for a hundred years without any scientific evidence to back it up," Cole says. "The egg on the face of the whole legal system would just be too great."

---

© 2004, The Orange County Register (Santa Ana, Calif.).

http://www.montereyherald.com/mld/montereyherald/news/nation/10026701.htm
 
when i took a forensics class, i was surprised to find that they didn't talk about exact print matches. they look for similarities, which were rated and given points. so many points meant a fairly likely match, but nothing was absolute. and DNA -- i don't know how all samples aren't contaminated.
 
Study Of Faulty Fingerprints Debunks Forensic Science 'Zero Error' Claim

While forensic scientists have long claimed fingerprint evidence is infallible, the widely publicized error that landed an innocent American behind bars as a suspect in the Madrid train bombing alerted the nation to the potential flaws in the system. Now, UC Irvine criminologist Simon Cole has shown that not only do errors occur, but as many as a thousand incorrect fingerprint “matches” could be made each year in the U.S. This is in spite of safeguards intended to prevent errors.

Cole’s study is the first to analyze all publicly known mistaken fingerprint matches. In analyzing these cases of faulty matches dating from 1920, Cole suggests that the 22 exposed incidents, including eight since 1999, are merely the tip of the iceberg. Despite the publicly acknowledged cases of error, fingerprint examiners have long held that fingerprint identification is “infallible,” and testified in court that their error rate for matching fingerprints is zero.

“Rather than blindly insisting there is zero error in fingerprint matching, we should acknowledge the obvious, study the errors openly and find constructive ways to prevent faulty evidence from being used to convict innocent people,” said Cole, an assistant professor of criminology, law and society.

The study appears in the current issue of the Journal of Criminal Law & Criminology.

Cole’s data set represents a small portion of actual fingerprint errors because it includes only those publicly exposed cases of mistaken matches. The majority of the cases discussed in this study were discovered only through extremely fortuitous circumstances, such as a post-conviction DNA test, the intervention of foreign police and even a deadly lab accident that led to the re-evaluation of evidence.

One highly publicized example is that of Brandon Mayfield, the Portland lawyer who was arrested and held for two weeks as a suspect in the Madrid train bombings in 2004. FBI investigators matched prints at the scene to Mayfield, and an independent examiner verified the match. But Spanish National Police examiners insisted the prints did not match Mayfield and eventually identified another man who matched the prints. The FBI acknowledged the error and Mayfield was released.

Wrongful convictions on the basis of faulty evidence are supposed to be prevented by four safeguards: having print identifications “verified” by additional examiners; ensuring the examiners are competent; requiring a high number of matching points in the ridges before declaring the print a match; and having independent experts examine the prints on behalf of the defendant. However, each of these safeguards failed in cases Cole studied. In fact, in four of the cases, independent experts verified the faulty matches.

Despite print examiners’ zero-mistake claim, Cole points out that proficiency tests conducted since 1983 show an aggregate error rate of 0.8 percent. Though that may seem small, when multiplied by the large number of cases U.S. crime laboratories processed in 2002, it suggests there could be as many as 1,900 mistaken fingerprint matches made that year alone.

“While we don’t know how many fingerprint errors are caught in the lab and then swept under the rug – or, worse, how many have still not been caught and may have resulted in a wrongful conviction – we clearly need a full evaluation of the errors,” Cole said. “The argument that fingerprints are infallible evidence is simply unacceptable.”

http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/20 ... 124509.htm
 
New Scientist had quite an in depth article on this IIRC around 18 months ago, you might be able to find it on their website if you're lucky.
 
NS

(This as much of the NewScientist article you can access, you need a paid subscription to get more)

Forensic evidence stands accused
31 January 2004
James Randerson Andy Coghlan
Magazine issue 2432
Despite a century of use, doubts remain over the reliability of using fingerprints to convict suspects
THE UK has been troubled this past week by revelations that flawed scientific advice given to courts may have led to the wrongful conviction of hundreds of men and women accused of harming their children.

More than 250 infant death convictions, and potentially thousands of child abuse cases, are to be reviewed after judges decided that the cases may have relied too heavily on controversial and conflicting medical theories.

However, a New Scientist investigation has discovered that other, potentially flawed, forensic assumptions are still routinely being accepted by the courts.

One such assumption is the supposed infallibility of fingerprint evidence, which has been used to convict countless people over the past century. Contrary to what is generally thought, there is little scientific basis for assuming that any two supposedly identical fingerprints unequivocally come from the same person. Indeed, according to a report published last month, the only major research explicitly ...

The complete article is 1506 words long.

http://www.newscientist.com/article.ns? ... 124321.100
 
Why we cannot rely on firearmforensics
23 November 2005
NewScientist.com news service
Robin Mejia

TYRONE JONES is serving a life sentence, in part because of a microscopic particle that Baltimore police found on his left hand. At his trial formurder in 1998 the crime-lab examiner gave evidence that the particle was residue from agunshot. He claimed Jones must have held or fired agun shortly before his arrest.

Jones denies this and still protests his innocence. His defence team is appealing the conviction, claiming that the science of gunsh ot residue (GSR) a nalysis is not as robust as the prosecution claims.

Now, a New Scientist investigation has found that someone who has never fired a gu n could be contaminated by someone who has, and that different criminal investigators use contradictory standards. What's more, particles that are supposedly unique to GSR can be produced in other ways.

Forensic scientists often testify that finding certain particle types means the suspect handled or fired a wea pon. Janine Arvizu, an independent lab auditor based in New Mexico, revie wed the Baltimore county police department's procedures relating to GSR. Her report concludes: "The BCPD lab routinely repo rted that gu nshot residue collected from a subject's hands 'most probably' arose from proximity to a discharging firea rm, despite the fact that com parable levels of gunsh ot residue were detected in the laboratory's contamination studies." The BCPD did not return calls requesting comment.

Some specialists argue for a more cautious approach. "None of what we do can establish if anybody discharged a fir earm," says Ronald Singer, former president of the American Academy of ForensicSciences and chief criminalist at the Tarrant county me dical examiner's office in FortWorth, Texas.

Peter De Forest of John Jay College of Criminal Justice in New York goes further. "I don't think it's a very valuable technique to begin with. It's great chemistry. It's great microscopy. The question is, how did [the particle] get there?"

GSR analysis is commonly used by forensic scientists around the world. In Baltimore alone, it has been used in almost 1000 cases over the past decade. It is based on identifying combinations of heavy metals in microscopic particles that are formed when the primer in acartridge ignites. The primer sets off the main charge, which expels thebullet.

There is no standardised procedure to test for GSR, but the organisation ASTM International, which develops standards that laboratories can look to for guidance, has developed a guide for performing the technique that was approved in 2001. This states that particles made only of lead, barium and antimony, or of antimony and barium are "unique" togunshot residue. The particles are identified using a scanning electron microscope and their composition analysed using energy-disp ersive spectrometry.

But recent studies have shown that a nonshooter can become contaminated without going near a firearm. Lubor Fojtásek and Tomás Kmjec at the Institute of Criminalistics in Prague, Czech Republic, fired testshots in a closed room and attempted to recover particles 2 metres away from the sh ooter. They detected "unique" particles up to 8 minutes after ashot was fired, suggesting that some one entering the scene after ashooting could have more particles on them than a sho oter who runs away im mediately (Forensic Science International, vol 153, p 132).

A separate study reported in 2000 by Debra Kowal and Steven Dowell at the Los Angeles county coroner's department reported that it was also possible to be contaminated by police vehicles. Of 50 samples from the back seats of patrol cars, they found 45 contained particles "consistent" with GSR and four had "highly specific" GSR particles. What's more, they showed that "highly specific" particles could be transferred from the hands of someone who had fired a g un to someone who had not. This doesn't surprise Arvizu. "If I was goin g to go out and look for gu ns hot residue, police stations are the places I'd look," she says.

Scientists using the techn ique are aware of the potential contamination problem, but how they deal with it varies. In Baltimore, for example, the police department crime lab's protocol calls for at least one lead-barium-antimony particle and a few "consistent" particles to be found to call the sample positive for GSR. The FBI is more cautious. Its protocol states: "Because the possibility of secondary transfer exists, at least three unique particles must be detected...in order to report the subject/object/surface 'as having been in an environment ofgunshot primer residue'." So a person could be named as a potentialshooter in Baltimore, but given the benefit of the doubt by the FBI.

Even worse, it is possible to pick up a so-called "unique" particle from an entirely different source. Industrial tools and fireworks are both capable of producing particles with a similar composition to GSR. And several studies have suggested that car mechanics are particularly at risk of being falsely accused, because some brake linings contain heavy metals and can form GSR-like particles at the temperatures reached during braking.

In one recent study, Bruno Cardinetti and colleagues at the Scientific Investigation Unit of the Carabinieri (the Italian police force) in Rome found that composition alone was not enough to tell true GSR particles from particles formed in brake linings (ForensicScience International, vol 143, p 1).

At an FBI symposium last June, GSR experts discussed ways to improve and standardise the tests. The bureau would not discuss the meeting, but special agent Ann Todd says the FBI's laboratory is preparing a paper for publication that "will make recommendations to the scientific community regarding accepting, conducting and interpreting GSR exams".

Singer maintains that the technique is useful if used carefully. "I think it's important as part of the investigative phase," he says, though not necessarily to be presented in court. But he adds: "There are people who are going to be a bit more, shall we say, enthusiastic. That's where you're going to run into trouble."

http://www.newscientist.com/channel/opi ... 4.300.html

Related Articles
Television shows scramble forensic evidence
http://www.newscientist.com/article.ns? ... 725163.800
09 September 2005
'Sperm clock' could pinpoint time of a rape
http://www.newscientist.com/article.ns?id=dn7079
05 March 2005
DNA duplication trick may lead to faster testing
http://www.newscientist.com/article.ns?id=dn6132
09 July 2004
Weblinks
American Academy of Forensic Sciences
http://www.aafs.org/
John Jay College of Criminal Justice
http://www.jjay.cuny.edu/
ASTM International
http://www.astm.org/
Forensic Science International
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/03790738
 
There are more interesting and relevant links at the url.

Forensic Science on Trial: The First of Two ScienceInsider Interviews
http://blogs.sciencemag.org/scienceinsi ... cienc.html
by Eli Kintisch

In February, a landmark report by the National Research Council (NRC) in February criticized nearly every aspect of the nation's forensics science system, including unreliable techniques for analyzing hair and DNA samples—a problem the U.S. Senate has been addressing in recent hearings.

But the NRC report said almost nothing about arson. So ScienceInsider conducted an email interview with John Lentini (left), a nationally known fire investigator who conducted an outside review of the controversial case of Todd Willingham, a convicted arsonist who was executed in 2004. Tomorrow, we will run an interview with Jay Siegel, a scientist who served on the NRC panel.

Q: What went wrong in Texas, and why? How widespread are such flaws in supposedly scientific forensic investigations?

J.L.: The only thing unique about Texas with respect to miscarriages of justice stemming from faulty arson convictions is that Texas is a profligate user of the death penalty, making it impossible to take back its harshest punishment. Actually, there are wrongful convictions for arson all over the United States. Because there is no DNA involved, and because the investigators only document the scene to the extent necessary to "prove" arson, it is exceedingly difficult to obtain a reversal of a wrongful conviction.

Most fire investigators were trained by mentors who were trained by mentors, who passed on belief systems based on anecdotal experience rather than on chemistry and physics.

Mr. Willingham's lawyers walked through the burned residence in Corsicana and were able to see the "pour patterns" on the floor. These were irregular areas of burning caused by the irregular nature of fire. In addition to the pour patterns, the fire investigators also saw evidence of "high temperatures at floor level," such as melted aluminum.

These investigators held the belief that fire burns up, and should not achieve temperatures equal to the melting point of aluminum at floor level. They're simply wrong. Fully involved compartments frequently achieve temperatures close to 2000°F at floor level, more than enough to melt aluminum. For some reason, fire investigators have adopted the notion that accelerated fires burn at higher temperatures than unaccelerated fires, where there is, in fact, no evidence to support this belief. It is, however, like most fire investigation myths, an appealing notion.

The same is true of the phenomenon known as crazed glass. No less an authority than the National Bureau of Standards [now called the National Institute of Standards and Technology] once published a Fire Investigation Handbook that came right out and said that craze class was an indicator of "rapid heating," though they never defined what they meant by "rapid." Actually, crazed glass only occurs when hot glass is rapidly cooled, but that did not stop many investigators from using this discredited indicator to send innocent citizens to prison.

There are numerous other myths involving artifacts found after fires, including spalled concrete, shiny alligator blisters, annealed bedsprings, and the "normal" angle of a V-shaped pattern. All of these myths were once taught as gospel by the National Fire Academy, which trained most of the fire investigators working in the public sector today. All of these myths have been debunked in the peer-reviewed literature since 1992, but a surprisingly robust rearguard of fire investigators has resisted accepting the loss of the "tools" that they use to determine fire causes and obtain convictions.

These convictions occur because juries believe the fire investigators who pretend to be scientists, but who do not even possess a minimal scientific education. The reason that these individuals are allowed to practice as fire investigators is that we, as a society, have asked them to do so, and have refused to offer sufficient salaries to attract college graduates.

The average fire investigator is a former fireman, who has studied chemistry and physics in high school. Don't get me wrong. These guys are heroes, but the skill set required to extinguish a fire is a completely different skill set from that required to investigate it. As firefighters, these individuals always succeeded in putting out the fire. They developed a mindset that did not allow for failure, and consequently, when the evidence required that a fire be called "undetermined," these individuals were unwilling to make that call.

Q: Is there something particularly challenging about arson investigations?

J.L.: One of the skills that is taught to fire investigators is a skill that just about anyone can learn—the ability to testify convincingly. This is easy to do when the witness believes what he is saying and believes that he has got the right suspect, even if the fire was an accident. Juries have no way of knowing that some of these determinations could have been more credibly made with a Ouija board.

Q: How would you fix the system?

J.L.: Since 2000, there has been a growing acceptance by the fire investigation community of the need for scientifically based determinations, and there have been a remarkable number of experts excluded from testifying in civil cases. Unfortunately, in criminal cases, exclusions of government experts are so rare as to make headlines. Judges do not have the skills to be gatekeepers when the question is one of science. The real gatekeepers in arson cases are the prosecutors.

One way to avoid wrongful convictions would be to hold the state responsible for proving beyond a reasonable doubt that a crime was actually committed before being allowed to introduce evidence of guilt. Most people wrongfully convicted are not pillars of the community, and it is not hard to get a jury to dislike them. If the first 2 weeks of an arson trial are about character assassination, then by the time the state presents its weak science, the jury doesn't care. Mr. Willingham was unfortunate in that his lawyers did not believe he was innocent, and did not put on an effective defense.

Ernest Willis was convicted of arson [in Texas] and sent to death row on almost identical evidence, but he had the "good fortune" of having such an ineffective lawyer that even the Texas Court of Criminal Appeals was forced to admit that he did not get a fair trial.

The National Academy of Sciences (NAS) report focused narrowly on "pattern evidence," which is usually used to associate the defendant with a crime scene. Very little mention was made of forensic science disciplines that answer the question, "What happened?" Fire investigation was mentioned in a single line. One can hope that when the Congress looks at the issues raised by the NAS report, they do not feel constrained to stay within its narrow scope when attempting to strengthen forensic science.
 
now fake fingerprints but go for the de lux job.

Chinese Woman Surgically Switches Fingerprints To Evade Japanese Immigration Officers
http://www.popsci.com/technology/articl ... n-officers
By Stuart Fox Posted 12.08.2009 at 12:16 pm 6 Comments


The First Line Of Defense No one will slip by with crack security guards like this James Bond-type watching our borders.
When attempting to evade biometric sensors, most go with the Tyler Durden or the John Doe from Se7en route, and simply cut or burn off their fingerprints. Unfortunately, that's a little obvious. So, for criminals looking to slip through fingerprinting in Japanese airports, fingerprint transplant surgery is all the rage.

That's what Lin Ring did when attempting to sneak back in Japan. Lin, a 27-year-old Chinese national, paid around $14,600 to have the fingerprints on her left hand switched with the fingerprints on her right hand. Customs agents discovered the fraud when they saw scars from the surgery on Lin's fingers.

While the BBC says this is Japan's first case of biometric fraud, the Mainichi Daily News claims that Lin is the eighth person arrested this year in Japan for altering their fingerprints in some fashion. According to Japanese police, Chinese doctors run a booming business in fingerprint surgery to help citizens slip past customs.

Lin was first deported from Japan in 2007; then, after she snuck back into the country with the fake prints, she was investigated for faking a marriage with a 55-year-old Japanese man. Customs agents discovered the phony fingerprints and she was deported again.

The lesson here: when getting surgery to defeat biometric sensors, shell out the extra dough for the high-class Helen Mirren package, not the low-rate Tara Reid job. It may cost a little more, but it's totally worth it.
 
Hair forensics could yield false positives for cocaine use

Hair analysis has become standard practice for determining whether someone has abused illicit drugs. But some experts have questioned whether current methods to wash away external contaminants from samples might affect test results. Now one team confirms that for cocaine detection, a pretreatment step can cause the drug on the outside of a hair shaft to wash into it and potentially lead to falsely identifying someone as a drug user. Their study appears in ACS' journal Analytical Chemistry.

Testing a person's locks for evidence of drug abuse has several advantages over urine and blood analyses. Sampling is simple and non-invasive. And a person's hair provides a record of use over a long period, whereas body fluids can only provide a short-term picture. However, it can be difficult to distinguish drugs incorporated into hair because someone has taken them from drugs that externally contaminate a non-user's hair when he or she was in the same room as the substances. To address this uncertainty, testers wash hair samples to get rid of any potential external contaminants. Eva Cuypers and colleagues wanted to find out if this step could affect the results.

The researchers followed standard procedures to wash off cocaine from non-users' hair. They then examined cross-sections of these samples and found that the drug had migrated into the hair shafts. The results suggest that current methods to decontaminate hair can have the opposite effect. The researchers conclude that this new insight could have implications for future hair analyses. ...

http://www.medicalnewstoday.com/releases/307369.php
 
Now they can be stolen - from photos!

Isao Echizen, a researcher at Japan's National Institute of Informatics, told a reporter from the Sankei Shimbun that he had successfully captured fingerprints from photos taken at 3m distance at sufficient resolution to recreate them and use them to fool biometric identification systems (such as fingerprint sensors that unlock mobile phones).

Echizen's research page doesn't provide any more details, and the English-language accounts do not provide links to the Japanese newspaper article, so details are sketchy. According to Agence France Press, the technique requires well-lit photos that are in focus, but does not appear to require special cameras.

The news hook for this is that flashing the peace-sign in photos -- as is common in Japan and elsewhere -- could expose your fingerprints. This is true! It's also true that cameras' resolution, sensor-speed, low-light sensitivity and autofocus capabilities are on the rise, so this is eminently plausible (after all, a fingerprint sensor is just a camera that takes pictures of your fingerprints). ...

https://boingboing.net/2017/01/12/moral-panic-japanese-girls-ri.html
 
(after all, a fingerprint sensor is just a camera that takes pictures of your fingerprints). ...
Many are. There were some that used RF fields to examine the ridge detail at the 'under skin' level but at least one of those was bought up by Apple. The latter are more secure as they not only look at a pattern, they can tell if it was a 'real finger' as well. The ones I looked at and repped for in the early 2000's could even tell a dead from a live finger, and you only had a small window between lopping off a finger and using it or it wouldn't work...
 
This new Smithsonian Magazine article reviews the long history of fingerprinting and the relatively recent realization it's not as reliable a personal identification practice as once assumed.

This is done in the context of illustrating the much shorter timeframe from introduction to criticism that has occurred with forensic DNA analysis.

The webpage includes a slideshow describing yet other forensic identification methods that are arriving on the scene.
The Myth of Fingerprints
Police today increasingly embrace DNA tests as the ultimate crime-fighting tool. They once felt the same way about fingerprinting

DNA identification has gone mainstream—from the elite labs of “CSI” to your living room. When it first appeared over 30 years ago, it was an arcane technique. Now it’s woven into the fabric of everyday life ...

Yet the DNA revolution has unsettling implications for privacy. ...

What happens to a society when there’s suddenly a new way to identify people—to track them as they move around the world? That’s a question that the denizens of the Victorian turn of the century pondered, as they learned of a new technology to hunt criminals: fingerprinting. ...

FULL STORY: https://www.smithsonianmag.com/science-nature/myth-fingerprints-180971640/
 
Many are. There were some that used RF fields to examine the ridge detail at the 'under skin' level but at least one of those was bought up by Apple. The latter are more secure as they not only look at a pattern, they can tell if it was a 'real finger' as well. The ones I looked at and repped for in the early 2000's could even tell a dead from a live finger, and you only had a small window between lopping off a finger and using it or it wouldn't work...

If you are that desperate you could always graft the finger on to a pig.
 
New research has demonstrated that fiber transfer does not necessarily require direct physical contact, overturning a presumption long held in forensic science.
Not Guilty! Forensic Research Proves That Textile Fibers Can Be Transferred Without Contact

Breakthrough forensic research at Northumbria University, Newcastle, has revealed for the first time that textile fibers can, under certain circumstances, be transferred between clothing in the absence of contact.

This new forensic discovery has not been demonstrated before and could have a major implication for fiber evidence in certain criminal cases.

Researchers within Northumbria University’s Department of Applied Sciences have proved that contactless transfer of fibers between garments can be possible through airborne travel.

Because it has largely been assumed that fiber transfer only occurs when two surfaces touch, it is generally accepted in a case that two surfaces have, at some point, been in contact with each other. However, researchers at Northumbria University have revealed that under certain conditions, this is not necessarily always the case.

“The results of the study were remarkable. It not only proved that textile fibers can transfer between garments in the absence of contact, but they can do so in relatively high numbers.”

In this study, the potential of fiber transfer between different items of sheddable clothing through airborne travel has been assessed for small, compact, and semi-enclosed spaces, such as elevators.

The results of this study demonstrate that when certain strict conditions are met (i.e. time, sheddability of garment, proximity and confined space), airborne transfer of fibers can occur in forensic scenarios, and that these could be in potentially significant numbers for fiber types, such as cotton and polyester.

The results of this study define a set of circumstances that can be used as a ‘baseline’ to evaluate the likelihood of an alleged activity being conducive to contactless transfer. ...

FULL STORY: https://scitechdaily.com/not-guilty...le-fibers-can-be-transferred-without-contact/
 
Fingerprints not so unique after all?

From "Law and Order" to "CSI," not to mention real life, investigators have used fingerprints as the gold standard for linking criminals to a crime. But if a perpetrator leaves prints from different fingers in two different crime scenes, these scenes are very difficult to link, and the trace can go cold.

It's a well-accepted fact in the forensics community that fingerprints of different fingers of the same person—"intra-person fingerprints"—are unique and, therefore, unmatchable.

A team led by Columbia Engineering undergraduate senior Gabe Guo challenged this widely held presumption. Guo, who had no prior knowledge of forensics, found a public U.S. government database of some 60,000 fingerprints and fed them in pairs into an artificial intelligence-based system known as a deep contrastive network. Sometimes the pairs belonged to the same person (but different fingers), and sometimes they belonged to different people.

Over time, the AI system, which the team designed by modifying a state-of-the-art framework, got better at telling when seemingly unique fingerprints belonged to the same person and when they didn't. The accuracy for a single pair reached 77%. When multiple pairs were presented, the accuracy shot significantly higher, potentially increasing current forensic efficiency by more than tenfold.

The project, a collaboration between Hod Lipson's Creative Machines lab at Columbia Engineering and Wenyao Xu's Embedded Sensors and Computing lab at University at Buffalo, SUNY, was published today in Science Advances. ...

https://techxplore.com/news/2024-01-ai-fingerprint-unique.html
 
This new Smithsonian Magazine article reviews the long history of fingerprinting and the relatively recent realization it's not as reliable a personal identification practice as once assumed.

This is done in the context of illustrating the much shorter timeframe from introduction to criticism that has occurred with forensic DNA analysis.

The webpage includes a slideshow describing yet other forensic identification methods that are arriving on the scene.


FULL STORY: https://www.smithsonianmag.com/science-nature/myth-fingerprints-180971640/
No mention of Mark Twains `Puddenhead Wilson?`
 
Back
Top